Nine Indicators to improve your chess (part 1)

by Jan Przewoźnik
9/22/2018 – In a deeply thought-out and specific proposal, IM JAN PRZEWOZNIK provides a method to train the way we think while playing a chess game. He unveils nine indicators that push us to reflect on our decisions and interprets them in terms of problem-solving cognitive strategies. The author laid out many example positions for everybody to test the method first-hand. In Part 1, we present the groundwork and the first three indicators.

ChessBase 18 - Mega package ChessBase 18 - Mega package

Winning starts with what you know
The new version 18 offers completely new possibilities for chess training and analysis: playing style analysis, search for strategic themes, access to 6 billion Lichess games, player preparation by matching Lichess games, download Chess.com games with built-in API, built-in cloud engine and much more.

More...

A method for broader and deeper thinking

In chess training, the thinking aloud method may be used. On the basis of such verbal reports, a chess player or his/her trainer may build a specific decision tree, containing all the moves considered. Then, from the decision tree, nine indicators may be abstracted and measured:

  1. Total number of moves considered in verbal protocol (M), 
  2. Number of successive solving propositions or number of fresh starts in decision tree (branches in decision tree - N), 
  3. Number of different options considered (A), 
  4. Number of successive changes in solving propositions (nn), 
  5. Reinvestigations of the move considered the most (Pmax), 
  6. The longest immediate reinvestigations of the move (Pser), 
  7. The longest variation (Dmax), 
  8. The value of the move chosen (V), 
  9. Thinking time (Time). 

These indicators can be interpreted in terms of problem-solving cognitive strategies. This article demonstrates the methodology of using nine indicators to analyse chess players' complex problem-solving processes.

It is recommended for the reader — at the beginning of the article — to solve the presented tasks himself and carefully write down his thoughts, especially the amount of variations (all the moves considered).

How does a chess player think?

Garry Kasparov, a former World Champion, was once called "a monster with a thousand eyes and from another age". In the phrase, Tony Miles paid tribute to his incredible ability to calculate broadly, deeply and quickly. On the other hand, in 1907 it was Jose Raul Capablanca who said: "I see only one move ahead, but it is always the correct one". This was the answer he gave to a question about his success in chess (Ross 2006).

Basler Zeitung, 15 May 1986, page 35

So where is the truth? Here and there. According to psychologists, there are at least two basic pillars in chess development:

  • Understanding chess positions in so called "chunks" (Chunking Theory by Chase and Simon, 1973).
  • Calculating variations effectively.

The first factor is strongly related to chess knowledge; the latter with operational skills.

These two basic pillars will be shortly analysed below. The aim of the article is to present the reader a certain method for psychological chess training.

Self-test positions

But first of all I offer the reader a chance to "think aloud" and write down all the variations in this position (Fig. 1). The value of the data obtained by the "thinking aloud" method depends largely on whether the subjects are able to freely and reliably verbalise their thoughts. Therefore, prior to proceeding with the experiments, the experimenter or trainer had to train the chess players to solve problems on the board, so that they would be able to simultaneously verbalise their process of thinking while working on the chess activity.

 

Compare your decision tree with the one drawn by a very strong master. His verbal comments are omitted here.

He discovers very soon the threat Rxf3 and the possibility of a sacrifice on d7:

Fig.2a. Sample decision tree, considered by the subject from Fig.1

He thinks White is winning. Suddenly, the defence 2…Qd6 comes to his mind:

Fig.2b. Sample decision tree, considered by the subject from Fig.1

So he starts the process which de Groot called "progressive deepening", a very important step in calculation (Fig. 2c)!

Fig.2c. Sample decision tree, considered by the subject from Fig.1

White has just discovered that 1.Bd7 Qxd7 2.Rxa7 Qd6! is losing and now is looking for other options (Fig. 2d): 

Fig.2d. Sample decision tree, considered by the subject from Fig.1

But still, the possibility of 1.Bd7 is tempting. However, he decides to play a more positional move (Fig. 2e):

Fig.2e. Sample decision tree, considered by the subject from Fig.1

The final choice is Qf4.

Therefore, 1.Bd7 Qxd7 2.Rxa7 Qd6! 3.Qxb7 Kd7 4.Qb5 Ke7 5.Rxc7 Qxc7 6.Qxd3 leads to Black's win. White has a real choice between 1.Qe2, 1.Qe4 or 1.Qf4.

Look at the possible variations and compare with your verbal protocol:

 

Of course, you may also use the following popular position, used by Adriaan de Groot. Then you will have the possibility to compare your thinking with verbal protocols by Alexander Alekhine, Max Euwe, Paul Keres, Savielly Tartakower, Reuben Fine… (de Groot, 1965; Fig. 3).

 

Look at the possible variations and compare with your verbal protocol:

 

So you have collected one or two protocols of your thinking. It is time to analyse them according to the Nine Indicators method.

The Nine Indicators method

In the field related to the methodology of chess psychology, the classic work of the Dutch researcher Adriaan de Groot still plays a leading role (de Groot 1965, 1981). The idea presented in this article refers to his concept, mainly as it is based on a "thinking aloud" method. I have been developing a method for broadening and deepening of thinking processes for years, by controlling the nine important indicators (see Przewoznik 2003, and Przewoznik, Soszynski 2001). 

Let us imagine the thinking process in a very popular study by Richard Reti (Fig. 4).

 

Of course, in all the examples the reader can analyse himself and save the solution first.

 

This may be presented as a process of some kind of "ideal" thinking. Look at the decision tree below (Fig. 5).

Fig. 5. Reti's study - the decision tree

Several quantitative indicators of individual ways of solving problems on the chessboard can be determined, namely: M, N, A, Dmax, nn, Pmax, Pser, T, W. These indicators were introduced by Adriaan de Groot (1965), in order to analyse "thinking aloud" protocols. In this article, only the names of indicators are modified, visualised and adapted to computerised statistical programs. These indicators give a wide range of opportunities for the study of thinking (Przewoznik & Soszynski 2001, Przewoznik 2003).

First of all, you may pay attention to the indicators in the decision tree, and then train them separately. When you decide what to do in a critical position and train your thinking, you may also ask yourself some adequate training questions. Examples are recorded below.

Indicator M

M - denotes the number of all considered moves contained in the entire decision tree. This can be used to measure moves in chess and also to analyse other decision-making situations. This indicator allows us to distinguish between those individuals who seek more information for decision making and those who make decisions on the basis of a smaller amount of information.

Possible questions: 

  • How much data do I have?
  • How much data do I need?
  • How many moves do I consider usually?

I often choose positions like the following, where the subjects have to calculate many and long variations if they want to find a solution. A fantastic example is the game played by my club friend Michal Krasenkow (Fig. 6).

 

Solution: 20.Rd3!! White had to check:

  1. 20… Qxd4 
  2. 20... Nxd4 21. Nd5 Qxb2 22. Nxe7+ Kh8 23. Qxh7+ Kxh7 24. Rh3#
  3. 20... g6 21. Nd5 gxh5 22.Nxc6 
  4. 20... Na5 21. Nd5 Bxd5 22. Nf5 exf5 23. Bxg7 Rfe8 (23...Kxg7 24. Qh6+ Kg8 25. Rh3) 24. Rh3 Nf8 25. Bxf8 Kxf8 26. Qh6+ Kg8 27. Qxh7+ Kf8 28. Qh8#.

Always winning.

The whole game is worth your consideration, as it included many strange and surprising moves:

 

Indicator N

N - denotes the total number of subsequent proposals to solve. This number may be a reflection of the subject's mentality type. A large value of N would be consistent with an empirical mentality type — a chess player who prefers a problem-solving approach based on the processing of large amounts of data and the calculation and verification of a large number of multiple variants. In contrast, a relatively smaller value of N may be characteristic of the theoretical chess player type, whose thinking is less empirical and more deductive, without specific variants. On the other hand, the value of N may be a function of the position on the chessboard, where the more complex a position is the greater the value of N, as more variations need to be calculated. 

Possible questions: 

  • How many branches do I usually calculate? Is it enough? 
  • How do I find out how much data I have left?

I often choose positions where the subjects have to count many but not too long variations if they want to find a solution (Fig. 7).

 

The whole game was used in the Soviet Union as a test to check the following skills: sense of the position, calculation of variations, originality, resourcefulness, sudden change in pawn formation, discipline of thought and the need to compare options (Zlotnik 1990).

Solution: Black had to calculate:

19…Qxc6 20.e5 Qc5+ 21.Kh1 with a lot of subvariations:

  • c6
  • Bf5
  • Qxe5
  • dxe5
  • Bxe5
  • Bd7
  • Be6

19…Bxa1 with a lot of subvariations.

19…Bxe5

 

Indicator A

A - denotes the set of alternative actions (candidate moves) considered by the subject, here understood as equal to the set of options in the decision-making process. In our example A = 6, since in the formula there appear six different solving propositions, a, b, c, f, g, h. Repetitions are omitted. The value of A may have a definitive psychological content: it can be associated with the fluency and semantic versatility of spontaneous thought (Guilford 1967). It can be assumed that persons characterized by great fluency and versatility would consider many different solving propositions in the process of choosing moves. It may be that fluency of thought will be more closely correlated with the value of N, whereas versatility more closely with the value of A. As in the case of N, the value of A may depend on the situation on the board and not only on the individual traits of the chess player.

Possible questions: 

  • Do I really explore the situation and problem broadly enough? 
  • Did I define the problem too narrowly? 
  • Did I define the problem too broadly?

Sometimes I choose positions so that the respondents have to look for a few options (called in Russia or the Soviet Union "moves-candidates") before they can come up with the right idea. The amount of possibilities must be small. I choose research models with three to seven options. Sometimes participants surprised me by offering new options (Fig. 8).

 

Solution: Samuel Lloyd gives the incredible variations starting with 1.Ke2! Brave the King! 

1.Ke2 f1=Q+ (1...Kxe4 2. Bd3+ Kd4 3. Rf4#; 1... cxd2 2.Rxf2+ Kxe4 3.Bd3#; 1... axb5 2.Rxf2+Kxe4 3. d3#) (1... Nc1+ 2. Ke3 f1=Q 3. Rxf1#) 2. Ke3.

One day in Miedzyzdroje, Izabela Siekanska (a master) discovered 1. Bd3+ Kd4 2. Rf3+ e5 (2... Be5 3. Bxe5#) 3. Rd5 mate!

 

The other six indicators and the explanation of "The Chunking Theory" will be published in Part 2 to follow shortly...


An International Master from Poland who was the Polish Champion in 1979, and also a psychologist and lecturer at The West Pomeranian Business School in Szczecin. He has written 25 books on chess and psychology of chess, communication, creative problem solving, selling skills, professional customer service, adult education, and social economy.

Discuss

Rules for reader comments

 
 

Not registered yet? Register