Has the number of draws in chess increased?

by Qiyu Zhou
3/21/2018 – People are generally worried about draws, which are often considered “boring”, where games may last up to six hours. To the common viewer (non-chess player), this may seem dull, and many believe this has led to a decline in sponsorship. But what do the statistics tell us? Using data now available, Qiyu Zhou, a remarkably young scientist has undertaken a thorough study of the numbers and provides a paper on her findings.

ChessBase 17 - Mega package - Edition 2024 ChessBase 17 - Mega package - Edition 2024

It is the program of choice for anyone who loves the game and wants to know more about it. Start your personal success story with ChessBase and enjoy the game even more.

More...

Draws in Chess over the Last 40 Years

A Statistical Analysis

Chess is largely an information game, and over the past 50 years, data (games of players) has become increasingly easier to access due to advancements in technology and personal devices. As such, is there a correlation between the number of top-level games and the number of draws over the course of the last 40 years? Is this due to the increasing number of games that are available for research?

As former U-14 world chess champion in chess, I often get asked this question by my friends or casual players. Many people are interested in the statistical aspects of chess, even if they do not play chess. The problem of draws often arises within the competitive chess-playing community, where I have heard a lot of complaints about how draws are too common, which makes viewing high-level games boring. When I play a game of chess, I know that there are three possible results, a white win, draw, or a black win. For a lot of sponsors and players alike (me included), chess is only really interesting if there is a clear winner. Personally, I only want to win. Thus, a common problem that has arisen in chess is that there is a lack of sponsorship because chess is often considered “boring”, when games may last up to six hours. To the common (non-chess player) viewer, this may seem dull. For this reason, some organizers have set up new types of tournaments and introduced faster time controls as an attempt to draw interest. Decisive games in strong tournaments are what catch people’s interests, what draws viewers.

Games that ended in draws can be picked out from the ChessBase database and analyzed. When choosing games to analyse, I selected them based on the strength of the players according to the Elo system. It can be assumed that anyone with an Elo of 2600 and above is very strong. The number of years was chosen based on the first decade (1970) in the 20th century in which a large number of games above an Elo of 2600 are available in Mega Database 2017. By ensuring the games used are of high quality, there can be at least some guarantee that these are the games common players are most interested in viewing. As tournament directors wish to ensure that their tournaments can receive sponsorship and viewership, it is important that the games do not all end in draws. 

In the early 20th century, collecting games and storing them became common practice for strong major tournaments. Initially, the only major tournaments that had their games retained were national championships of countries with a strong chess culture at that time (such as Germany, the USSR, Yugoslavia), Interzonals, World Championships and Zonal qualifiers. Thus, there are not many games available for research from the early 1900s.

Nowadays, all tournaments that are affiliated with FIDE (the World Chess Federation) require the recording and inputting of games, which leads to large amounts of information available for research and public consumption. Because there are millions of games in the database, to ease research and to produce results based on only the strongest tournaments (as these attract the most attention), I believed that only games of the world’s top players — where both are above an Elo of 2600 — will be looked at. However, it is important to note that the lack of high-rated games before 1970 can be attributed to the fact that ratings were not in general use yet, and by no means indicates there were not strong games played.

The time frame of the collected games used for this analysis will be from 1971 to August 3rd, 2017. The games are taken from Mega Database 2017, with updated games starting from December 2016 from The Week In Chess. It is important to note that I did not select games of a specific time control — thus after faster time control games became mainstream (think blitz and rapid championships) there may have been more decisive games, thus affecting the draw rates.

Data

To prove my point about how the numbers of recorded games have gone up, the bar chart below shows the total number of games collected per year. Please note that, at the time of writing, 2017 had not yet ended, which explains the low bar in 2017.

graph number of games

Total number of games per year, from 1970 to the start of August, 2017, by players who both have an Elo above 2600 (click or tap to enlarge)

pie chart of results

Result Games Percentage
White Win 22640 28.85%
Black Win 14121 18.00%
Draw 41697 53.14%
Total 78468 100%

The table above shows an overall summary of all the games used in this analysis. Out of the total number of 78,468 games played by players who both have an Elo above 2600. This is illustrated more clearly in a pie chart of the same data.

As can be seen, the majority of games are drawn. There are various reasons for this. Due to the fact that only top-level games were considered, most of the time, both players are fairly evenly matched, which results in equal play and an eventual draw. Likewise, players at the top level are more likely to play “safe”, meaning they will try to play for a draw from the very start, as they have more to lose. There are also many ways for a draw to occur, such as the three-fold repetition, which, according to Article 5 of the Official FIDE Handbook, means “the game may be drawn if any identical position is about to appear or has appeared on the chessboard at least three times.” Other ways to draw include, “agreement between the two players during the game. This immediately ends the game” as well as the "fifty move rule": “if each player has made at least the last fifty consecutive moves without the movement of any pawn and without any capture.” The number of rules that can cause a draw makes it more likely for the result to happen.

Let’s take a look at the draw rates per year, based on games where both players had an Elo above 2600. Please note that games that did not have inputted ratings have been omitted because a search function by Elo was used.

Year Total Draw
%
  Year Total Draw
%
1971 180 96 53.33%   1995 2134 1084 50.80%
1972 132 78 59.09%   1996 2380 1278 53.70%
1973 166 106 63.86%   1997 2342 1270 54.23%
1974 204 132 64.71%   1998 1133 601 53.05%
1975 252 180 71.43%   1999 1049 561 53.48%
1976 128 96 75.00%   2000 1430 819 57.27%
1977 238 146 61.34%   2001 1319 746 56.56%
1978 172 102 59.30%   2002 1567 812 51.82%
1979 186 116 62.37%   2003 1320 737 55.83%
1980 274 186 67.88%   2004 1897 984 51.87%
1981 286 174 60.84%   2005 2218 1264 56.99%
1982 294 176 59.86%   2006 2335 1244 53.28%
1983 312 208 66.67%   2007 3102 1630 52.55%
1984 382 268 70.16%   2008 3271 1812 55.40%
1985 276 170 61.59%   2009 3901 2006 51.42%
1986 386 212 54.92%   2010 3741 1997 53.38%
1987 466 246 52.79%   2011 3719 2063 55.47%
1988 754 482 63.93%   2012 3893 2077 53.35%
1989 926 538 58.10%   2013 4677 2392 51.14%
1990 706 408 57.79%   2014 4732 2282 48.22%
1991 1088 558 51.29%   2015 4917 2480 50.44%
1992 1382 734 53.11%   2016 5429 2642 48.66%
1993 2036 1068 52.46%   2017 2363 1316 55.69%
1994 2350 1120 47.66%          

Table of drawing percentage per year

The above table shows the total amount of games played and the rates for draws between the start of 1971 to early August 2017. At first glance, it is evident that the number of recorded games has been gradually rising, although there appears to be no real correlation between the number of draws and the number of games played per year. The data is displayed in a more coherent manner in the scatter plot below:

scatter plot of drawing percentage by year

(Click or tap to enlarge)

Firstly, before 1993, the relationship between the percentages of drawn games is insignificant compared with year, as it fluctuates between 52% to 70%. However, after 1993, the rate stays at around 50%. The data is further analyzed using the least squares regression formula, to determine any correlation after the year 1990. This is relevant because, as seen before, there is not a lot of data (refer to the raw data table in the appendix for specific numbers of games collected) between the years before 1993. This signifies that the percentage of draws may be related to the number of games available for analyzing. The number of games collected from before 1993 is lower due to many reasons, such as the lesser number of people playing, as well as the number of organized tournaments. Nowadays, tournaments are organized much more frequently.

Calculations

Calculations are useful in the real world as they can provide statistical evidence for correlation, averages, and the significance of the relationship of data. For chess, in this case, a line of best fit can visually show the correlation between two variables, which in this case will be the percentage of games drawn to the year.

Least Squares Regression

To find the line of best fit, statistically, the least squares regression formula is used.

y=ax+b

where:

y is the predicted value
a is the slope
b is the y-intercept

Taking the table from above and assuming a linear correlation:

y = –0.0031x + 6.7835

percentage of games that were drawn

Taking a closer look at the linear regression line starting from the 1990s, in which the number of games went up drastically, it can be seen that the percentage of draws has been fairly even. A reason for the horizontal line for draw rates may be that computers became more popular, and tools for analyzing chess started to appear. Thus, games played became more accurate, with both sides at top levels making fewer mistakes, leading to an even number of draws. In this situation, the slope of the line is given by:

y = –0.0005x + 1.55

percentage of draws by year

It turns out that the average is fairly close to the line of best fit given by this scenario. This graph once again shows that the number of games collected has not caused the number of draws to rise.

Average

The average is calculated by adding up all the data and then dividing by the total number of data points. The average is convenient for looking at what is the overall percentage of draws and compare each year in relation. In this case, to calculate the average percentage of draws per year:

where:

is the mean or average,

is the sum of all the percentages

and n is the total number of years.

Using the data from the table of raw data above, since

= 2674.08% and n = 47

= 56.8953%

The average number of draws per year can be rounded to 56.90%. This leads to standard deviation, which can be used to further analyse any correlation between the numer of draws.

Standard Deviation

In statistics, standard deviation (s) is a measure of the dispersion of the data from the mean. It will be used in this case to calculate how close each data point is to the mean. It is expressed as:

where

is the mean or average;

x is each individual score;

n is the number of data points;

∑ is the sum of the values; σ is the standard deviation.

A high standard deviation means that the data point is spread out, while a low one indicates that each data point is close to the average. The calculated standard deviation in this situation is 6.3025%, which means that each data point is fairly close to the average. This means that even with more games played, there has been no noticeable rise in the number of draws each year.

Though the previous graphs and data analysis did not prove the hypothesis of the number of top-level games and the number of draws, over the course of the last 40 years and an increasing number of games that are available for research, an interesting trend can be seen in the following graph:
 

If we graph the number of games collected in a year against the percentage of games that were drawn for that specific number of games collected, we can see clearly that draws are the most common results. However, it is important to look at the y-axis and see the number of collected games for each draw rate. As it turns out, the lower the number of games collected each year, the higher the percentage of draws. This graph appears to be exactly opposite to the hypothesis; increasing the number of games that are available for research has, in fact, lowered the percentage of games drawn. However, it can be assumed that a lack of data may be the reason for the high number of draws.



Quite evidently, the higher the number of games played, the higher the number of draws. This is proven by the linear line of best fit, which shows a direct linear correlation. The previous graph supports the conclusion drawn from this one, which was that an increasing number of games available for research lowered the percentage of games drawn.

Conclusion

While there appears to be no correlation between the number of games played and the number of draws that occur at high-level chess, draws are still the most frequently seen result in the game of chess. Whether or not tournament directors will be able to change this through shorter time controls or other methods is up for debate. However, it can be assumed that only when there is a lack of information, there is a significantly high number of draws at the highest levels. For the years 1971 until the early 1990s, not a lot of games were collected. While there is a high number of draws, it cannot be assumed that the number of games collected is the cause of the number of draws, because of the lack of data. However, from the 1990s onward, the relative percentage of draws each year stabilizes to around 50%. We can, therefore, conclude that with a higher number of collected games, there will be a more consistent number of draws.

I concluded that there is no correlation between the number of top-level games and the number of draws over the course of the last 40 years. Nonetheless, further research could be conducted on the draw rates of lower level players, which would provide an interesting contrast to the data of higher-level players, because at lower levels games are more likely to have a definite result due to the frequency of mistakes. As players in chess are only becoming stronger, it can be assumed that the rate of draws will stay close to the current prediction of 50%.

Acknowledgements

I am very grateful for the review and feedback I received from Dr. John Nunn, Ken Thompson and Jeff Sonas. Their comments encouraged me to look deeper into the reasons for the trends and to draw a more fitting conclusion about the situation of draws in chess.

Links

About the author

WGM Qiyu Zhou [pronounced Chee-you Jo], born in 2000, is a Canadian chess player who has competed for team Canada at the Women's Chess Olympiad since 2014 and who won the Canadian women's championship in 2016.

Qiyu learned to play chess at the age of four in France. In late 2004 the family moved to Finland, and Qiyu won the Finnish Youth Chess Championships five times (in 2005, 2007, 2008, 2009 and 2010) in the U10 Open section. Also in 2010, she won the Nordic School Chess Championships in the U11 Open division in Sweden. In 2008, she won the silver medal in the U8 Girls section at the World Youth Chess Championship in Vung Tàu, Vietnam.

In 2011, Qiyu transferred chess federations from Finland to Canada. She won the Canadian Youth Chess Championship in 2012 and 2013, in the Girls U-12 and Girls U-14 sections respectively. She won the Girls U-14 World Youth Championships in Durban, South Africa, 2014.

Also in 2014, Zhou made her debut at the Women's Chess Olympiad in Tromsø, Norway. She played board four for the Canadian team scoring 6½/9 points. In the same year she also took part in the World Youth Under-16 Chess Olympiad in Gyor, Hungary playing board four for team Canada, which finished fifth. She finished first in the U-18 Girls category at the North American Youth Chess Championships in 2015 Toluca, Mexico. As a result, she was automatically awarded by FIDE the title Woman International Master (WIM). In September 2016, Zhou won the Canadian women's championship and as a result qualified to play in the Women's World Chess Championship 2017. You can watch a speech she did on how to achieve one’s goals.


WGM Qiyu Zhou [pronounced Chee-you Jo], born in 2000, is a Canadian chess player who has competed for team Canada at the Women's Chess Olympiad since 2014 and who won the Canadian women's championship in 2016.

Discuss

Rules for reader comments

 
 

Not registered yet? Register

IlkkaS IlkkaS 3/25/2018 01:01
First of all the analysis doesn't take into account the rating inflation. A person with 2600+ rating in 1970 was a top 10 player, while nowadays that rating in enough maybe for top 200. Secondly comparing classical time control games to blitz and rapid games just seems silly. Isn't it obvious quick games are decisive more likely? And thus doesn't the draw rate have a strong correlation to the the ratio of quick games analysed compared classical time control games?
wb_munchausen wb_munchausen 3/24/2018 02:20
I like the idea used in a tournament recently where wins and draws were not scored as 1 and 1/2, but were weighted differently (I forget the exact scoring) to give greater incentive to play for a win. There is no change to the rules of chess, but simply the scoring.
Baneour Baneour 3/24/2018 01:31
What an interesting and well written article.
jaberwocky jaberwocky 3/23/2018 06:19
More faster time control chess tournaments may be a good idea, because:
(1) They might have less drawn games.
(2) Perhaps TV stations would be more likely to broadcast such events.
(3) Some older players don't like long games.
ketchuplover ketchuplover 3/23/2018 03:28
I think it'd be nice to know if the average number of moves per draw has changed over time.
koko48 koko48 3/22/2018 10:52
@Bobbyfozz Yes, Mamedyarov-Caruana was anything but boring. It was a real, fighting draw

Contrast this with the Grischuk-Karjakin game this round, or the Ding-So game. In the latter game over 30 moves were played (the game would meet 'Sofia Rule' requirements) but the game never deviated from the equal path

Were these 'GM draws'? Perhaps not in the literal sense, they were both a little too long. Were they boring? Yes. Were they non-games? Yes, or close to it. Both players seemed content with a draw and neither player really tried to deviate from the equal path
Bobbyfozz Bobbyfozz 3/22/2018 09:44
I was watching Mamedyarov vs Caruana today. Anything but dull and it ended in a draw. Mamedyarov taking it to K v K was boring, but Caruana was trying (and so was Mamed) to take it to the limit because they were the top competitors. I don't have money like some of the sponsors purport to have, but what they did was real chess. Aronian fell apart. This happens, the will to go on, is gone. That may be the aspect of chess which can't be learned or taught. It comes down to HOW BAD do you want the prize. When I was younger our favorite "work" expression was: "Ya gotta wanna!"Revenge is good for stirring the pot. So is helping yourself or someone else out of a dangerous spot. These are all forms of "Ya Gotta Wanna." Fischer and Alekhine, to cite 2 really wanted to go for it. In 2000 when Kasparov threw in the towel against Kramnik, he "didn't want it bad enough." He had already forecast Kramnik as a future world champion. There are times Garry where you just have to keep quiet.
Jack Nayer Jack Nayer 3/22/2018 03:37
Fine analysis.

Draws are not a proxy for boring.
Kasparov and Karpov produced many draws and many were not boring at all.
To me, the match in NY was the most boring WC of all time.
The only way to quantify this is to take a sample of chess amateurs with a decent rating and let them evaluate the degree of boring/excitement using a score.
koko48 koko48 3/22/2018 02:48
This is a nice analysis and this young lady may have a promising career in the expanding field of data analysis and data science.

Unfortunately though, it doesn't address the core issue. The problem is not draw rate as a whole, but the preponderance of non-games.... Unplayed, GM draws

A well fought, fighting draw is a good chess game. If all of these draws were of the 'real,' fighting variety nobody would have a problem with that. Most chessplayers are aware that a 'real' game among two strong players, well played on both sides, should end in a draw more often than not. That is simply the nature of chess.

Parsing these statistics to weed out the GM draws is a trickier exercise. You can try to separate them based on number of moves (shorter draws tend to be non-games) but that wouldn't be entirely accurate because some short games are 'real' games, and many non-games are 30 moves or longer. The latter case is not an obvious or literal 'GM draw', but both players are happy with a draw and neither player is striving or doing much to break the equilibrium.

Whether the rate of non-games has gone up over the past 40 years or not, also isn't the core issue. The core issue is that, regardless of where it's trending, there are far too many of them. And there is no doubt that turns many potential followers off from the game
Carpalim Carpalim 3/22/2018 09:10
I think the ELO cut off of 2600 might be a problem.

The FIDE january 1971 rating list (http://fidelists.blogspot.no/2008/01/january-1971-fide-rating-list.html) had only 16 players rated above 2600, and the difference between number 2 (Spassky) and the last players was only 90 ELO-points. Most pairings of players rated above 2600 would be of players with approximately the same rating. Players with approximately the same rating should be expected to have approximately the same playing strength, and the share of draws should be expected to be high.

Today 234 players are rated at 2600 or above and of these 55 are rated at 2690 or above, which means that the playing strength of players rated above 2600 are more unequal today than in 1971. (The standard deviation has increased from 41.6 to 50.5, and excluding the No. 1 players (Fischer and Carlsen) from 26.7 to 49.1) With more unequal playing strength within the group of 2600+ players, the share of draws is expected to fall.

I think that in addition to the ELO cut off of 2600, the study should only include games between players with approximately the same playing strength, for instance by only including games where the rating difference between the players are 50 ELO points or below.
fons3 fons3 3/22/2018 08:25
@ melante, hillion

Agreed, the problem of rating inflation affecting the result should have been avoided.

Also rapid and blitz games should have been excluded as those obviously skew the results, especially considering there are more rapid and blitz events these days.

Even classical time controls have become faster leading to more mistakes and less draws.

The current Candidates are spectacular, but all we have to do is look at the qualifiers for these Candidates:

Sharjah 2017 - 74.1% draws
Moscow 2017 - 70% draws
Geneva 2017 - 61% draws
Palma 2017 - 73% draws
Strength In Numbers Strength In Numbers 3/22/2018 07:57
I'd say the type of tournament is a very important factor too. It would be logical for the draw rate to be higher in a match than in a Swiss open. Would this explain the higher draw rate in the 1970s, where a significant part of the small number of games was played in Candidates and World Championship matches?
geraldsky geraldsky 3/22/2018 06:43
White to play and draw. How to draw in this difficult position by Black? Draws are always part of the game. There is also beauty in a draw.
Claudioarrau Claudioarrau 3/22/2018 03:19
An aversion to draws is ultimately an aversion to logic as the basis of chess. You can huff and you can puff about lack of fighting spirit, but the fact remains that if both players play the most logical moves, the game must end in a draw.

If you find that boring, you should switch to dice games or card games.
melante melante 3/22/2018 02:21
@hillion : that's a fair point. The study should likely take into consideration the top 200 players in the world (for example) regardless of rating to avoid this potential issue.
hillion hillion 3/22/2018 01:00
Due to ELO rating inflation, only 10 players were rated over 2600 in the 70's. Today they are approximately 200! This explains of course the increase of the number of recorded games, but does not at all allows any comparison between the beginning and the end of the period.
fixpont fixpont 3/22/2018 12:18
@RoselleDragon: i would say under 40 instead of 20, i still prefer the Sofia rules, i think the guiding principle should be the following: if the game is a draw then play until the last spectator in the audience understands it why it is a draw, becasue like every sport is played and (ultimately) sponsored the audience (the public), i would like to see fighting spirit in every single game
RoselleDragon RoselleDragon 3/21/2018 11:54
Draws in chess are fine. Only non chess players can't find beauty in a well fought game that happens to end up in a draw. What is not OK is Grandmaster draws; meaning, when draws are agreed in games under of 20 moves.
Offramp Offramp 3/21/2018 11:14
I have no problem with draws. Players are getting better and better. The games they play are interesting even if the end result is ½-½.
1